from
QuantaMagazine Website
for Quanta Magazine A specific set of neurons deep in the brain may motivate us to seek company,
holding social species
together.
As social animals, we depend on others for survival.
Our communities provide mutual aid and protection, helping humanity to endure and thrive.
Early humans, for example, could take down large mammals only by hunting in groups.
But how did these powerful communities come to exist in the first place?
John Cacioppo proposes that the root of social ties lies in their opposite - loneliness (which is different from 'solitude'). According to his theory, the 'pain' of being alone motivates us to seek the safety of companionship, which in turn benefits the species by encouraging group cooperation and protection.
Loneliness persists because it provides an essential evolutionary benefit for social animals. Like thirst, hunger or pain, loneliness is an aversive state that animals seek to resolve, improving their long-term survival.
If Cacioppo's theory is correct, then there must be an intrinsic biological mechanism that compels isolated animals to seek out companionship. Something in our brains must make it feel bad to be alone and bring relief when we're with others.
Researchers at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT) think they've found the source of that motivation in a group of little-studied neurons in part of the brain called the dorsal raphe nucleus.
Stimulating these neurons drives isolated mice to find friends, according to research (Dorsal Raphe Dopamine Neurons Represent the Experience of Social Isolation) published earlier this year in the journal Cell.
The finding provides critical support to Cacioppo's theory and illuminates a deep connection that links specific structures in the brain to social behavior.
The new study - the first to link specific neurons to loneliness - is part of a growing effort to map out the genetics of social behavior and its underpinnings in the brain.
Together, Cacioppo's work and the new findings from MIT are helping to move loneliness from the realm of psychology and literature to biology.
Social Creatures
Gillian Matthews stumbled across the loneliness neurons by accident.
In 2012 she was a graduate student at Imperial College London who had been studying how cocaine changes the brain in mice. She would give the animals a dose of the drug, place each one alone in a cage, and then examine a specific set of its neurons the next day.
She did the same for a control group of mice, injecting them with saline instead of cocaine.
When Matthews returned to her mice 24 hours after dosing them, she expected to see changes in their brain cells, a strengthening of neuronal connections that might help explain why cocaine is so addictive.
To her surprise, both the drug-treated mice and the control mice showed the same changes in neuronal wiring.
Overnight, the neural connections onto a certain set of cells had grown stronger, regardless of whether the animals were given drugs or not.
The brain cells she was interested in produce dopamine, a brain chemical typically associated with pleasurable things.
Dopamine surges when we eat, have sex or use drugs. But it does more than simply signal pleasure.
The brain's dopamine systems may be set up to drive the search for what we desire.
The researchers focused on dopamine neurons in a brain region called the dorsal raphe nucleus, best known for its link to depression. (This may not be a coincidence - loneliness is a strong risk factor for depression.)
Most of the neurons that reside there produce serotonin, the chemical messenger that drugs such as Prozac act on.
Dopamine-producing cells make up roughly 25 percent of the region and have historically been difficult to study on their own, so scientists know little about what they do.
Matthews speculated that other environmental factors during the experiment might have triggered the changes. She tested to see if simply moving mice to new cages altered the dopamine neurons, but that couldn't explain the effect.
Ultimately, Matthews and her colleague Kay Tye realized that these brain cells were responding not to the drug but to the 24 hours of isolation.
Mice, like humans, are social creatures that generally prefer to live in groups.
Isolate a mouse from its cage mates, and once confinement ends it will spend more time interacting with other mice, to a much greater extent than if it had been with its mates all along.
To better understand the role the dorsal raphe neurons play in loneliness, the researchers genetically engineered the dopamine cells to respond to certain wavelengths of light, a technique known as optogenetics. They could then artificially stimulate or silence the cells by exposing them to light.
Stimulating the dopamine neurons seemed to make the mice feel bad. Mice actively avoided stimulation if given the choice, just as they might avoid physical pain.
Moreover, the animals appeared to enter a state of loneliness - they acted like they had been alone, spending more time with other mice.
Spectrum of Loneliness
Cacioppo first formally proposed his evolutionary theory of loneliness a decade ago.
Strong support comes from the fact that our sensitivity to loneliness is heritable, like height or risk of diabetes - about 50 percent of an individual's level of loneliness can be tied to their genes.
The evolutionary theory for loneliness,
Indeed, like diabetes, people have varying degrees of susceptibility to loneliness.
In evolutionary terms, it's helpful for a population to have some variability in this trait. Some members of a community would be,
Mice also show this variability.
In Matthews' experiments, the most dominant mice - those that win in fights against their cage mates and have priority access to food and other resources - show the strongest reaction to having their loneliness neurons stimulated.
At those times, the highest ranking animals search out companionship more fervently than animals on the lowest rungs of the social ladder.
These mice also avoid stimulation of the loneliness neurons more avidly than the lower ranking members, suggesting that the dominant mice find it more unpleasant. The lowest ranked mice, in contrast, didn't seem to mind being alone.
Perhaps they enjoyed isolation, being free of their harassers.
Tye and Matthews' findings suggest that these dorsal raphe nucleus neurons help to resolve the disconnect between the level of social connection the animal has and the level it wants. Imagine loneliness as a desire for ice cream - some animals love ice cream and some don't.
The dopamine neurons drive the ice cream lovers to seek out the dessert but have little effect on everyone else.
The varying reactions suggest two intriguing possibilities:
Perhaps some animals are wired from birth to crave social contact.
These animals then seek out others and become aggressive as they try to maintain their position in the group, eventually attaining top status. Alternatively, certain mice may start out with an aggressive personality, picking on other animals in their group.
The brain wiring in these animals might change as a result, driving the mice to seek out others to bully.
Tye and Matthews are planning additional experiments to distinguish those two possibilities.
Cacioppo said he almost "fell over" when he saw Tye and Matthews' results. He's done extensive research on loneliness in humans, using brain imaging to identify parts of the brain that are active when people feel lonely.
But brain imaging has a coarse resolution and can't analyze specific cell types like Tye and Matthews did in mice.
Tye and Matthews' research helps to reframe loneliness from a state of profound despair to a motivational force encoded in our biology.
|